Serendip is an independent site partnering with faculty at multiple colleges and universities around the world. Happy exploring!
Knocking on New Doors :A Review of Mind Wide Open
Fortunately for
many of us, studying neurobiology doesn’t have to be rocket science. In fact, it’s
merely brain science! According to Steven Johnson, author of Mind Wide Open, studying the brain is
best approached with an open mind (pun intended). At times, it may be difficult
to comprehend the various complex processes which are occurring within our
skulls each day. Furthermore, it is often a challenge to relate such knowledge
to our everyday lives. Johnson’s book attempts to make this journey a little less
arduous, however, by colloquially explaining several salient brain processes in
a way that inexperienced readers can understand and appreciate without much
confusion.
As the author explains,
understanding the brain requires us to learn a unique language about
neurobiology, and throughout his book, Johnson acts as both translator and
teacher to improve our communication of ideas and theories underlying brain
processes. By integrating this specialized grammar into our daily lexicon, we
are able to discuss important concepts and raise interesting questions that
prompt us to continue researching and generating theories related to the brain.
In this manner, Johnson familiarizes his readers with 200 pages of text on
neurobiological processes by incorporating his own experience and interest with
celebrated research to create a comprehensive manuscript that scratches the
surface of the multi-layered subject matter, bringing us along on what winds up
being a remarkable exploration through the brain. Of course, Johnson is
incapable of answering all questions related to the biology of the brain in his
short book, but just as our Biology 202 class laid stepping stones for students
to begin their journey to understanding the mind, Mind Wide Open likewise encourages us to further examine lingering
questions by providing us with a set of keys to unlock unopened doors
Throughout the semester, a
particularly salient topic for our class regarded the I-function. What is it?
How does it work? Is it necessary? Do animals possess it? These are just a
handful of the many questions we sought to answer as the semester progressed.
Johnson appears to have come up with these same questions. In Mind Wide Open, Johnson describes his
research experiences related to the I-function, though he does not provide the
same term to define this mysterious mechanism. The author includes a variety of
examples related to the significance of the I-Function, several of which we
have touched upon and debated in class. However, just as our class served to
bring us closer to the truth without spoon-feeding us answers, Johnson likewise
could not simply explain the I-Function without posing more questions. In
particular, the author uses examples that many of us can relate to such as fear
and attention. Imagine walking down a dark alley way, for instance. Although
you are (presumably) safe as you imagine this scenario, part of you may feel
uneasy or afraid. But, “which you is you?” Johnson asks (51). Or, in other
words, which part of you is afraid and which part knows it is safe?
Additionally, which of these parts is the real
you and is it in any way linked to the I-Function? Johnson’s text leads us to
ask such additional questions in order to bring us closer to the truth without
ever flat-out providing answers. Though this may be frustrating for some
readers, I personally found great enjoyment interacting with the text through
such a unique form of dialogue in which I could respond to Johnson’s questions
with my own questions and vice versa. This style of writing was not only
impressive, but also encouraging because as each conversation developed over
chapters, I began to feel more knowledgeable and prepared for future
discussions.
Despite the potentially frustrating
aspects of Mind Wide Open’s absence
of distinct answers, I found the book to be inspiring and entertaining because
of its relevance to everyday life, which I can very much appreciate since at
times it can be difficult to link brain complexities to the real world. By far,
I most enjoyed reading Johnson’s chapter entitled Survival of the Ticklish because the contents explored an
interesting subject-matter that I have always had an interest in, yet we did
not have sufficient time to discuss in class this semester: laughter. Admittedly,
at first glance, laughter does not appear to have any substantial link to
neurobiology. Yet Johnson was sure to provide evidence falsifying this naïve
assumption. According to the author’s extensive research, laughter has an
evolutionary purpose. Upon reading this, I wondered how humor could possibly
pertain to survival of the fittest. To my surprise, Johnson explained that
“laughing is not an instinctive physical response to humor the way a flinch
responds to pain or a shiver to cold,” (120) as I once assumed. Instead, it is
“an instinctive form of social bonding that humor is crafted to exploit.” In
other words, the roots of laughter are not cultivated in humor; instead, laughter
functions, at least in part, to strengthen relationships. As I read on, I
realized that this makes perfect sense considering we often laugh with loved
ones and rarely laugh with strangers. In fact, laughing with strangers is what
transforms acquaintances into friendships. Johnson additionally describes a
relevant research finding that people are 30 times more likely to laugh when in
the company of others than when alone supporting the concept that laughter is a
construct of social bonding. Indeed, there is a biological reason explaining
why laughing is contagious. On a different note, being one to always laugh at
my own jokes (perhaps more than my audiences) I was interested in learning what
it is about my sense of humor that drives me into a fit of laughter at the
sheer mention of the word “fart.” Piecing together the information Johnson
published with what I know about the kinds of things that make me laugh, I was
able to discern what should have been obvious: Apparently, laughing is the
brain’s response to unexpected or surprising circumstances. By autonomously
facilitating such situations using my I-Function, like mentioning a particular
bodily function, I am able to amuse my unsuspecting, more rational self which
of course would never dream of producing such inappropriate banter. Perhaps
this is the same reason why puns and metaphors are so entertaining to some
people, such as myself, and it likewise explains why babies find the game
peek-a-boo so hysterical. Johnson’s description of laughter sheds light on what
makes us chuckle at sudden one-liners, just as when a mother’s unexpected
exclamation “Peek-a-boo!” is enough to send a baby into a flutter of giggles. Survival of the Ticklish concludes by
declaring that in constructing the comical situations mentioned above, laughter
enables the cementing of social connections.
Another significant area of research
regarding brain functioning is related to the role hormones play in influencing
our behavior. Johnson devotes one of his chapters to this interesting, yet
sometimes overlooked subject matter. I was delighted to draw parallels between
his research and my own when studying how the “love hormone,” oxytocin, affects
social bonds. In my last web paper I attempted to explain the mechanisms at
work when experiencing romantic and maternal love. Johnson’s discussion
followed this same path, but with a few interesting twists. For example, the
author described that hormones levels unique to humans are in part what make us
more monogamous beings than say rabbits. This discussion led Johnson to reveal an
interesting finding that I had not come across in my own research on the role
of oxytocin in facilitating relationships. Apparently, prairie voles are a
distinct species that mate for life, and do so because of specific oxytocin
levels which produce emotional connections between mates. Relating back to his
discussion on the I-Function, Johnson conversely explains that reptiles do not
possess the emotional responses that are so characteristic of us humans.
Johnson presumed that reptilian lack of emotion may stem from the absence of a
neocortex, which is where our class inferred the I-Function was located. By
assimilating this information with that which we discussed in class, I began to
imagine the circuitous relationship among hormones, the I-Function, and
emotions. As per usual, Johnson’s text led me to pose new questions about such
an intricate circuit, and though I could not draw definitive conclusions from
the author’s content, I genuinely feel more knowledgeable about the subject
matter than I had previously been before reading Mind Wide Open.
What’s going on up there? This is
the question that prompted me to enroll in Biology 202 in hopes of extracting a
core basis of knowledge about brain functioning. Obviously there is much more
to learn about the brain than can be discussed in a semester-long course, but
our class’ continuing conversation over the past few months has paved the way
for increased interest in this area of study by provocatively answering
questions with new questions in such a way that each open door leads us to open
new doors. Though we have discovered that no such door will ever hold an
ultimate answer, there is something to be said about the fact that each door
leads to new destinations which subsequently bring us closer to an
understanding of how our minds work. I
am proud to say that this mind-opening class is what led me to read the inspiring
book by Steven Johnson. Mind Wide Open functioned
as one such door that subsequently guided me to many others that I hope to
unlock and explore in the future.
Johnson, Steven, (2004). Mind Wide Open: Your Brain and the Neuroscience of Everyday Life. Scribner: New York
Comments
Mind Wide Open